Representation of Information in Large-Scale Outdoor Spaces Using Reference Frames

Document Type : Original Article

Author

Assistant Professor of Architecture, Najafabad Branch, Islamic Azad University, Najafabad, Iran

Abstract

Reference frames (or frames of reference) show how the knowledge of object organization is formed in the environment by the physical movement of the person or the environmental conditions where the representation takes place. This paper examines the formation of reference frames in memory to represent the spatial structure of large-scale outdoor environments, due to the lack of consideration in the research background, the lack of attention to the scale of the formation of reference frames, and the lack of ecological validity in other studies. For this purpose, two types of tests are used in this paper to change the scale of the environment as well as pointing targets. In one test, familiar and unfamiliar participants are introduced to the positions of the three building triads, by walking the path that encompasses each group. Then, maps are presented to them along with the representation of these three groups in five different directions (0-180 degrees), and they judge whether each triple group is correctly represented based on the relative positions of the buildings. In another test, participants are introduced to the position of eight unknown objects in a pedestrian-oriented environment, moving in two pre-programmed and inscribed paths (one path aligned with the sidewalk and the other misaligned, at a 45-degree angle). Then, participants use their memory, pointing to objects as targets. In both tests, the dependent research variables are the pointing accuracy and the latency in responding to the examiners’ questions. The results show that the positions of unfamiliar buildings are subjectively represented as egocentric reference frames. The allocentric frame of reference is defined by the environment when the environment is familiar, and the representation also tends to allocentric, as humans become familiar with the environment. On the other hand, object positions are subjectively represented in regular environments and distinctive by the geocentric frame, but are selected based on egocentric experience.

Keywords


Ball, K., Birch, Y., Lane, A., Ellison, A., & Schenk, T. (2017). Comparing the Effect of Temporal Delay on the Availability of Egocentric and Allocentric Information in Visual Search. Behavioural Brain Research, 331, 38-46. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.018
Blajenkova, O., Motes, M.A., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2005). Individual Differences in the Representations of Novel Environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(1), 97-109. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.12.003
Boer, L.C. (2002). Mental rotation in perspective problems. Acta Psychologica, 76(1), 1-9. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(91)90050-A
Dehghan, N. (2019). Wayfinding Strategies in Interior Architecture, Case study: National Library of Iran. Armanshahr Architecture & Urban Development, 11(25), 81-95. http://www.armanshahrjournal.com/article_85068.html
Evans, G.W., & Pezdek, K. (1980). Cognitive Mapping: Knowledge of Real-world Distance and Location Information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(1), 13-24. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.1.13
Foley, J.E., & Cohen, A.J. (1984). Working Mental Representations of the Environment. Environment and Behavior, 16(6), 713-729. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916584166003
Hintzman, D.L., O’Dell, C.S., & Arndt, D.R. (1981). Orientation in Cognitive Maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 149-206. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(81)90007-4
Hund, A.M. (2016). Visuospatial Working Memory Facilitates Indoor Wayfinding and Direction Giving. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 233-238. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.008
Iachini, T., & Logie, R. (2003). The Role of Perspective in Locating Position in a Real- world, Unfamiliar Environment. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 715-732. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.904
Iachini, T., Ruggiero, G., & Ruotolo, F. (2014). Does Blindness Affect Egocentric and Allocentric Frames of Reference in Small and Large Scale Spaces? Behavioural Brain Research, 273, 73-81. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.07.032
Klatzky, R.L. (1998). Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections: Springer-Verlag.
Levinson, S.C. (1996). Frames of Reference and Molyneux’s Question: Crosslinguistic Evidence. In Language and Space. 109-169. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.
Li, D., Karnath, H.O., & Rorden, C. (2014). Egocentric Representations of Space Co-exist with Allocentric Representations: Evidence from Spatial Neglect. Cortex, 58. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.06.012
McNamara, T.P., Rump, B., & Werner, S. (2003). Egocentric and Geocentric Frames of Reference in Memory of Large-scale Space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 10(3), 589-595. doi:https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196519
Mou, W., & McNamara, T. P. (2002). Intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 162-170. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.162
Ohtsu, K. (2016). Spatial Learning by Egocentric Updating during Wayfinding in a Real Middle-scale Environment: Effects of Differences in Route Planning and Following. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 50. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.12.006
Paillard, J. (1991). Brain and Space: Clarendon Press.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1997). The Child’s Conception of Space: Routledge.
Proulx, M.J., Todorov, O.S., Taylor Aiken, A., & de Sousa, A.A. (2016). Where am I? Who am I? The Relation between Spatial Cognition, Social Cognition and Individual Differences in the Built Environment. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(64), 1-23. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00064
Richardson, A.E., Montello, D.R., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Spatial Knowledge Acquisition from Maps and from Navigation in Real and Virtual Environments. Memory & Cognition, 27(4), 741-750. doi:https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211566
Richardson, G. (1981). Spatial Representation and Behaviour Across the Life Span: Theory and Applications, Lynn S. Liben, Arthur H. Patterson, Nora Newcombe (Eds.). Academic Press, Cambridge (1981). Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1, 247–251. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-4944(81)80043-6
Roger, M., Bonnardel, N., & Le Bigot, L. (2011). Landmarks’ Use in Speech Map Navigation Tasks. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(2), 192-199. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.12.003
Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., McNamara, T.P., Shelton, A.L., & Carr, W. (1998). Mental Representations of Large and Small Spatial Layouts Are Orientation Dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 215-226. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.1.215
Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T.P. (2001). Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43(4), 274-310. doi:https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0758
Shepard, R.N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-dimensional Objects. Science, 171(3972), 701-703. 
Siegel, A.W., Krasic, K.C., & Kail, R.V. (1978). Stalking the Elusive Cognitive Map. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Children and the Environment. 223-258. Boston, MA: Springer US.
Waller, D., Montello, D.R., Richardson, A.E., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Orientation Specificity and Spatial Updating of Memories for Layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1051-1063. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1051
Werner, S., & Schmidt, K. (1999). Environmental Reference Systems for Large-scale Spaces. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1(4), 447-473. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010095831166